Podchaser Logo
Home
The Mind Numbing Medicine

The Mind Numbing Medicine

Released Tuesday, 7th February 2023
Good episode? Give it some love!
The Mind Numbing Medicine

The Mind Numbing Medicine

The Mind Numbing Medicine

The Mind Numbing Medicine

Tuesday, 7th February 2023
Good episode? Give it some love!
Rate Episode

Episode Transcript

Transcripts are displayed as originally observed. Some content, including advertisements may have changed.

Use Ctrl + F to search

0:00

This is the BBC. This

0:03

podcast is supported by advertising outside

0:06

the UK. Hello,

0:08

Curios. Another show. Penultimate

0:10

one of this particular run,

0:12

and it's a very mysterious one today.

0:14

Another one in the category of stuff

0:16

that don't really understand.

0:18

mean, kind of giving away the entire episode there,

0:20

Adam? Sure. It's not a secret. It

0:24

is Mavo that we knock

0:26

people out with drugs and don't

0:28

know how we're doing it.

0:29

So this is an episode we should explain. It's

0:31

about and aesthetics and how they work.

0:40

Today's investigation is an absolute

0:42

knockout literally.

0:43

Yes. By the end of this, you should be blissfully

0:46

unaware of the pain you've been because we had a

0:48

question coming from two different listeners.

0:50

Alicia Nissan, a train driver in New South

0:52

Wales Australia, and Neil Morton from

0:55

Sterling in Scotland, and they both

0:57

emailed curious cases at bbc dot co dot

0:59

u k asking how do anesthetics

1:01

work? I know this one. Someone in a white

1:03

coat inject you and you wake up

1:05

handcuffed to an arangotang in the cargo hold

1:08

of a plane on its way to

1:08

Jakarta. Case solved. Pretty

1:11

sure that was your stag too that you're describing

1:13

there. Now, how do they actually

1:15

work? So how what's going on

1:17

in the body? Have you ever had one, by the way? I

1:19

have in anger. No. Not

1:21

that. In

1:22

a hospital environment, yes. Just once,

1:24

I loved it. I absolutely loved it. I

1:26

see something I was doomed to have taken out

1:29

the sensation as the

1:32

stuff runs up your arm and then waking up

1:35

several hours later, not with 4

1:37

teeth short and not knowing what the hell of harm.

1:39

I know you've had one -- Mhmm. --

1:41

because you phoned

1:43

me. Video called me

1:46

about five minutes after waking up. To

1:48

tell me about something to do with work.

1:51

Not a hundred percent coherently. I have to

1:53

confess. Yeah. I mean, that's I mean,

1:55

things that I think you're in your true estate. When

1:57

you're when you're coming up from random

1:58

aesthetic. My true estate is workaholic.

2:01

I'm

2:02

coherent, workaholic, working, workaholic.

2:04

Is that alright. Well, let's make things a little bit

2:06

less in coherent because we have two experts

2:09

joining us today. We have doctor Fiona Donald,

2:11

who's president of the Royal College of Anesthetists.

2:14

Yeah, that qualification seems

2:15

appropriate. And we also have Anil Seth,

2:17

professor of Cognizant neuroscience at the University

2:19

of Sussex and general consciousness

2:22

guru. And let's with

2:24

you. Being under

2:26

a general anesthetic, that is not

2:28

the same as being

2:30

asleep. Right? So what is the

2:32

difference? It really isn't the same. I've

2:34

had a number of general anesthetics. And

2:36

what's always struck me is how different it

2:38

is. When you go to sleep, and

2:40

you wake up again. There's always a sense

2:42

of some time having

2:43

passed. You know, you know, it's roughly been

2:45

a few hours. But

2:47

under Anna sees you, It's not

2:49

just the experience of absence. It's

2:51

the absence of any experience.

2:53

You could have been under for five minutes

2:55

five hours or or fifty years even.

2:57

The the ends just join

2:58

up, you're gone, and then you're back.

3:01

It's

3:01

a it's a kind of preemination of the

3:03

of the oblivion that might have

3:05

been there before you were born or maybe after

3:07

you

3:07

die. Goodness. That, I mean, that is quite a profound

3:09

thought for right at the top of curious cases, but let's

3:11

go with it. So we're not gonna spend

3:13

much time talking about the nature of consciousness because

3:15

that's a whole series in

3:17

itself. But it's the absence of

3:20

self. You do. You see you you kinda

3:22

cease to exist. I think that's right. I think

3:24

you just go away entirely. The

3:26

brain Now it's still going, it's

3:29

still doing stuff. Your body is still alive. It's

3:31

it's really, I think it's one of the best inventions

3:33

ever. And it just turns

3:35

the self off, but more than the it turns off

3:37

any experience and experience of

3:39

the outside world, and of course, the experience

3:41

of the surgeon cutting into your

3:43

body, which is why it's such a brilliant invention.

3:45

And it's also when you need someone

3:47

like Fiona to be in the

3:49

room with you. Fiona, I know that you you

3:51

do a lot of work dishing out the drugs But

3:53

have you ever undergone a genoa and set it yourself?

3:56

Yes, I have. Just once again, I agree.

3:58

It is really different to going

4:00

to sleep. And you just lose that

4:03

time completely. And the other thing

4:05

is that so at the end

4:07

of my operation, when I was in the recovery

4:09

room, my parents had a long conversation with the

4:11

anesthetist, and we discussed

4:13

many things of which I have no

4:15

memory. I've I've really enjoyed

4:17

the last gentleman in Caesar I had

4:19

because it wasn't 4 an emergency operation,

4:21

it was a planned

4:22

surgery, a relatively minor one.

4:24

And I still remember talking to the

4:26

anesthesiologist on way in and asking

4:28

him all sorts of probably for him unexpected

4:31

questions about exactly what what I was

4:33

getting and what the timescale was going

4:35

to be. And

4:35

then really trying to pay attention

4:39

to what happened at the moment that the

4:41

anesthetic

4:41

went. And Adam, you mentioned that you feel that

4:43

cold.

4:44

That will So that really

4:45

really weird sensation and shit, isn't it

4:47

from the inside? But now it doesn't mean that you

4:49

as a professor of consciousness must be singularly

4:52

the worst patient and an

4:54

anesthetist wants to meet on the way

4:55

in. Tell me everything about

4:58

what's happening. You know what? The

5:00

anesthetist always has a last word because

5:02

I can just be gambling on, but they

5:04

they turn the tap on and undergone. Now

5:07

count back backwards. Oh, bye

5:08

bye. Fiona, do they actually do the count backwards

5:10

thing? Because I they did it with me and I remember thinking

5:12

I'm gonna do this and I got to about seven.

5:14

I witnessed my son having some surgery

5:16

one

5:16

time, and they asked him to count backwards from ten,

5:19

and he didn't get to ten.

5:20

I don't know. I've I don't have to say,

5:22

I just tend to sort chat in mainly

5:25

to people as they're

5:27

drifting off just to sort of have a kind of reassuring

5:29

noise in the background. I've not used

5:31

the number of things, but I think people do still do

5:33

it. And and we always win, as

5:35

I know

5:35

says. Yeah. You

5:37

always win. I think I think the last time I had,

5:39

it was definitely I think I

5:41

got about three numbers in before

5:43

oblivion took

5:44

over. I mean,

5:45

people people try and count really quickly. It's

5:47

not a problem.

5:49

The ecosystem. But you've been I mean,

5:51

that conversation when you come around, there is

5:53

something kind of interesting about that.

5:55

Do people say mad

5:57

stuff? Do people generally come up

5:59

with a

6:00

blank blank blank 4 that? Bong

6:01

crew's ideas as they're coming around.

6:03

They

6:04

I think they might be a little

6:06

bit more distributed than than

6:08

normal. But in general, they

6:11

make sense. And so that sort of lulls you into a

6:13

4 sense of security that you think you're having

6:15

a really good conversation with

6:17

them. And what

6:18

what do we

6:19

know about the pharmacology of what is actually

6:21

going on? That that that loss of self that

6:23

Anna was describing there. Do we do

6:25

we understand what's going on

6:28

in terms of the drugs? So, yeah, I

6:30

don't think we've got a full understanding of,

6:32

you know, how every drug drug works,

6:34

but we do know quite a bit.

6:36

So there's been there's been a lot of theories over the

6:38

years, but I think the sort of most

6:40

recent theories are that is around ion

6:43

gated channels and

6:45

that's gabot channels. So gabaries

6:48

gamma aminobutary acid. And

6:50

it's probably just – it's probably not just one

6:52

thing. But in general, what's happening is

6:54

that by interacting with those receptors,

6:56

they're kind of reducing the amount

6:58

of excitation matter transmission

7:01

of signals essentially so

7:03

that you then lose

7:04

consciousness. I okay. I'm

7:06

just I mean, I heard some stuff about ions.

7:08

I heard some stuff about receptors, but

7:10

what I did hear there was that people

7:12

aren't sure about how this

7:15

thing works. Have I have I understood that correctly

7:17

that people have been up general aesthetics

7:19

without knowing what they're doing.

7:21

So I wouldn't be quite like that.

7:24

I I would say that we generally know, you know,

7:26

we know how they're gonna work, and we know the effects taken

7:28

to have and the side effects and how to manage

7:30

things. But I think the

7:32

complete exact mechanism of

7:34

exactly how general anesthetics work

7:36

hasn't been completely worked at yet.

7:38

Okay. Let let me let me see if I've understood

7:41

this then. So in your brain, you've got

7:43

Nuance firing around all over the place.

7:45

And you want to get something

7:47

that stands in the way and stops the

7:49

receptors picking up on those

7:52

neurons

7:52

firing. Think that's the sort of thing.

7:54

So what it does is blocks a

7:57

receptor where that transmission might occur

7:59

and thereby causes the effect

8:01

to get and that

8:02

happens, as I say, at the gamba receptors. And

8:04

we also know that it happens at NMDA

8:06

receptors. Yeah. No. I do wanna ask you about,

8:08

like, just the practicalities of it

8:10

because all four of us. And I expect many

8:12

of the listeners have had general anesthetics at some

8:14

point. What happens?

8:17

You know, how do you apply the drugs? What

8:19

happens after we go

8:20

under? So we first of all have

8:22

to pop a little tube

8:25

into one of the veins so that we can

8:27

give the drugs into the veins because most of the

8:29

drugs that we give going through the through the veins,

8:31

and people would just gradually

8:33

drift off to sleep. And then you

8:35

can either keep people asleep by

8:37

getting them to breathe gases that will

8:39

keep them asleep, or you can

8:41

continue with the drugs through the vein.

8:43

And then at the end, you just stop the

8:45

drugs and then they wake

8:46

up. It's magic. It's basically

8:49

magic. It just sounds a bit like magic. I mean, what

8:51

you're what you're describing here is essentially

8:53

it sounds like you put pumps of magic of veins then

8:55

and then magical things happen. But

8:57

okay. The drugs that you're giving, this cocktail

8:59

of drugs that you're giving, do you also

9:01

have to Are there

9:03

different physiological things that you need to

9:05

control? I'm thinking about swallowing and breathing

9:08

while you're under. That's a really

9:10

interesting question. And people will

9:12

generally continue to breathe under

9:14

anesthetic for themselves unless

9:16

you paralyze their muscles so

9:18

then they can't breathe and then you have to take

9:20

over their breathing. So you're going

9:22

to take over that breathing and we

9:24

have ventilators that we use,

9:26

but a lot of anesthetics are done

9:28

with people just continuing to breathe

9:30

for themselves. What about swallowing? What about

9:32

sort of clearing your

9:35

throat of saliva? So people

9:37

don't generally swallow under anesthetic.

9:40

Because you're at a a kind of deep level

9:43

of loss of consciousness such that

9:45

you you wouldn't necessarily swallow. If people

9:47

are swallowing, might be assigned

9:49

that they're a bit

9:51

lighter under the anesthetic than you would

9:52

like. Okay. So

9:53

did people ever wake up? Does that ever

9:56

happen? You know, if you haven't been there where someone

9:59

So this, again, is an important

10:01

issue in anesthesia, and we talk

10:03

about accidental awareness under general

10:05

anesthesia. And that's

10:07

where someone is unintentionally awake.

10:10

Now, that doesn't happen very

10:12

often at all, and we have

10:14

and monitoring that we can use. So we have

10:17

clinical monitoring where we're looking at the patient because

10:19

we're always with the patient when they're

10:21

asleep. But we also have brain

10:23

monitors that we can use to look at the brain

10:25

activity, and that gives us a very good

10:27

idea of whether the anesthetic

10:29

is deep

10:29

enough, not too deep because, of course, you don't want to

10:32

give people too much answer to either. Must

10:34

be a very serious thing to to

10:36

to need to avoid. I mean, I imagine quite

10:38

life if somebody were to wake up while they're

10:40

having major abdominal surgery.

10:42

It's not so much that it's life threatening,

10:45

it's it's horrible

10:48

and it it can lead to

10:50

post traumatic stress --

10:51

Mhmm. -- for sorts

10:52

of very unpleasant sequelae.

10:54

So obviously, we do everything

10:56

we can to avoid

10:57

that. And as I say, it is very rare. So

10:59

I want

10:59

the public to be reassured. Quite

11:02

much more terrifying than waking up in a

11:04

house or not being able to So I was wondering

11:07

whether you were gonna mention that because that's the

11:09

other terrifying thing about, you

11:11

know, these these sort of incidents of

11:13

awareness during anesthesia, which is that

11:15

the person may often be

11:17

paralyzed as well, so not able

11:19

to demonstrate that they're, in

11:21

fact, awake. Which is I think some of

11:23

the older and Fiona, you can correct me if I'm wrong, but I

11:25

think some of the older methods here involved

11:28

basically putting a turn a k on so

11:30

that the parallel, the kura

11:32

Ray didn't affect one

11:34

limb so that if the person did wake

11:36

up, they would be able to you

11:37

know, wave with their left hand to

11:40

say that that I'm awake. Yeah. The

11:41

isolated forearm 4. Yeah. Yeah.

11:43

The

11:43

isolated forearm technique. Yeah. So

11:45

what this so hang on. Let me make sure I understand

11:47

this then. So historically, I

11:50

mean, back when people didn't know how

11:52

anesthetics worked, you could

11:55

be conscious, but paralyzed, physically

11:58

physically placid, but

12:01

but but totally awake in your mind. And

12:03

unable to signal to surgeons and those around

12:05

you that that this was going

12:06

on. And so how long ago we talked?

12:09

I mean, you you said this is a it was like a standard

12:11

thing,

12:11

Fiona. I mean, I've never seen the

12:13

isolated form technique. So it's an ancient, you

12:15

know, it's a historical

12:16

technique. The most

12:19

serious forms of awareness under general anesthesia, which

12:21

are extremely rare rare, are the ones

12:23

where people are completely

12:25

awake, but paralyzed.

12:28

Now that does still happen

12:29

-- Mhmm. -- and that is a generally

12:32

a medical error, but

12:34

it's very, very rare. And the fact that we've

12:36

got all the machinery that we've got now that makes

12:38

it much, much less likely to

12:40

happen

12:40

now. And of course, I mean, that's

12:43

I don't wanna trivialize this too much, but, of course,

12:45

your job is not done as soon as the person

12:47

is is is knocked out. You don't sit there,

12:49

you know, playing solitaire on your phone for the rest

12:51

of the surgery. You're monitoring

12:54

four changes in in

12:55

physiology. Right? That's right. So before

12:58

we start, we wire

13:00

people up to lots of monitors to look at

13:02

all their physiological variables,

13:04

heart rate. Their oxygen

13:06

levels, their blood pressure. And

13:08

obviously, what a surgeon is

13:10

doing will have an effect on that as well as what

13:12

we're giving. So sometimes we need

13:14

to give drugs to help keep things

13:16

like the blood pressure normal or sometimes

13:18

we just need to give a little bit more pain

13:20

killer or something like that.

13:20

You mentioned their fioner about not

13:24

giving too much anesthetic. I

13:27

mean, imagine these these drugs are pretty

13:29

potent, but But what would happen if

13:31

you were to give somebody too much? Does it

13:33

affect when they wake up? So the

13:35

main anesthetic that we use is a

13:37

drug called pro perform. I mean, all

13:39

antipsychotics generally will drop the blood pressure

13:41

anyway, but it could drop the blood pressure more than you'd

13:43

like. It might slow down

13:45

someone's breathing more than you'd like.

13:47

But equally, we know that particularly in the elderly

13:49

in the frail or people who are

13:52

generally unwell, those

13:54

effects can be exacerbated. And

13:56

so you really want to make sure that you're

13:58

in that sweet spot where you're giving

14:00

enough anesthetic to make sure

14:02

that they're obviously asleep during

14:04

the during the

14:05

operation, but not causing Jimmy's

14:07

side effects. You're

14:08

walking a tight rope then it sounds like

14:11

we're skilled professionals, and we

14:13

do this a lot. And and so, actually,

14:15

it's not as much as a type on a type of as

14:17

it sounds. But we do, you know, we do need to

14:19

pay very close attention to what's going

14:21

on. I mean, I I can't I still can't

14:23

quite get over the idea. That an

14:26

entire career in administering drugs

14:28

to people and still not really be totally

14:30

sure about the ways in which it's

14:31

working. Yeah. I can I can I

14:34

can see where you're coming from? I mean,

14:36

I think the thing is that they have

14:38

a consistent way of

14:40

working So when you give the drug, it has a consistent

14:42

way of working. And so you you

14:45

recognize that that is happening. So I'm not

14:47

sure that you actually need

14:49

to know the molecular basis

14:51

of what's happening in order to be able to

14:53

use those drugs. I mean, there are lots of

14:55

things we do in life where we don't know the

14:57

the full mechanism of of why

14:59

why someone's

15:00

working. You

15:01

could still play ball sports before Newton came

15:03

along and explain gravity to us. Absolutely.

15:09

So obviously, one of the roles of

15:11

the police statistics is to is

15:13

to monitor physiology and that includes monitoring

15:15

brain activity as Fiona mentioned. Now

15:17

we're gonna delve deep into the brain now

15:19

because we wanna know what's going on in the

15:21

brain during a general

15:21

anesthetic. And what

15:24

that can tell us about awareness

15:26

and alertness and how

15:28

consciousness normally works. And to kick us

15:30

off, we spoke to an old friend of yours,

15:32

Adam. Irene Tracy. You remember her? Yeah.

15:34

Wow. She's no friend of

15:35

mine. Oh, come on

15:36

now. Irene is a professor of

15:39

anesthetic neuroscience at Oxford University.

15:41

And listeners might remember

15:43

her because she runs a

15:45

little torturelobe and

15:47

she stabbed and electrocuted and

15:49

burnt Adam with

15:50

Chili's. 4, nothing we

15:53

can't get we can't get sign off or anything. No. I

15:54

think there was a heat gun involved in the chill shot. Okay. Anyway,

15:57

it was all part of our episode on

15:59

Pain. And I seemed to

16:01

recall. You quite liked it. Well, wait. I mean,

16:03

a little bit. Okay. Anyway,

16:05

we expect Irene about an exciting

16:07

experiment that she did. To

16:09

explore how patterns of brain activity

16:11

change as doses of anesthetic

16:13

increase and that involved

16:15

putting people in little EEG caps

16:17

like swimming cats with electrodes

16:18

inside, as well as looking at

16:20

their brain activity in a

16:22

scanner. We actually did what we call a multimodal

16:25

imaging experiments, so we had people with those

16:27

swimming cap on the e g kit,

16:29

and we put them inside

16:31

our large brain imaging scanner. Whilst

16:33

the subjects have been bombarded with

16:36

painful stimuli, an auditory stimuli.

16:38

So we were seeing how the brain was reacting to

16:40

that. But what we did then was

16:42

started to infuse in the anesthetic agent,

16:44

propofol, but instead of just squirting it

16:46

in, we very, very slowly gave

16:48

the anesthetic. So instead of taking them into

16:50

deep unawareness in fifteen

16:52

seconds, we took about thirty minutes to take

16:54

them down. Allowing

16:56

each person's brain to react

16:58

to that increasing dose of anesthetic

17:00

and to switch off in the way

17:02

that that brain wanted to. Now I've

17:04

spent thirty years looking at people's brains

17:06

and I know that people's brains

17:08

are very different and the brain is what

17:10

we're switching off during general anesthesia.

17:13

And so what we were looking for in the analysis

17:15

was, are there common things that are

17:17

happening to everybody in terms of how

17:20

the brain switching off and other things that are unique to each

17:22

person's brain. So

17:24

we stumbled across this incredible

17:26

serendipitous finding that

17:28

when people were having

17:30

an increasing dose of anesthetic. The

17:32

dose was carrying going up or pop up. And as

17:34

it went up, everybody mounted these slow

17:36

waves. And then suddenly, one person

17:38

would reach a limit and they wouldn't make any more.

17:40

And even though the dose of anesthetic kept on

17:42

going up, that was it for

17:45

them. And then the next subject did exactly the same,

17:47

but maybe they reached a different level that was

17:49

maybe a little bit more or a little bit less.

17:51

But when they reached their own

17:54

individualized maximum level of slow

17:56

waves. We realized the brain had gone

17:58

into what I call perceptual

18:00

unawareness. It was sort of lights

18:02

out. So even though the signals were coming

18:04

in, they were not being routed to the right

18:06

bits of the brain. So we're very excited

18:08

about this as a potential new device

18:10

that we could create that could really

18:12

help anesthetist deliver in

18:14

a more titrated and bespoke

18:16

way, just the right amount of anesthetic.

18:18

Okay, Anne or there's a lot going on there and a

18:21

lot to unpack from my

18:23

friend Irene. So basics

18:24

first. When she talks about mounting slow

18:27

waves, what what what what is

18:29

Irene talking about? She's

18:31

talking about these very slow oscillations

18:33

in overall brain activity.

18:35

The brain is always oscillating.

18:38

It's it's activity flows, it comes and it goes, it goes up and it

18:40

goes down, and it does this at various

18:42

different frequencies. There's

18:44

the alpha rhythm, which is very

18:46

prominent usually over the

18:48

visual cortex at the back of the brain.

18:50

This is an oscillation at about ten

18:52

hertz, ten cycles per second.

18:54

But what Irene is talking about are

18:56

delta oscillations. These are much

18:58

slow at about one to four

19:00

cycles per second. And what

19:02

she observed in her as a brilliant experiment

19:04

just to infuse the anesthetic very

19:05

slowly, is that these

19:08

delta waves, these very slow

19:10

waves those become much more prominent

19:12

and this hints at an underlying

19:14

mechanism for anesthesia and loss

19:16

of consciousness in

19:18

general. Which which has been called cortical by

19:20

stability. And what does that mean? That just means

19:22

that the the brain can be in

19:24

two different states. It can be in its normal

19:27

active states. And then it can be in very

19:29

quiescent, very low level, not much

19:31

going on state. And basically, these

19:33

these slow waves, these delta oscillations,

19:36

they show that the brain is periodically,

19:38

you know, a few times every second, just

19:41

entering this state of a very

19:44

little

19:44

activity. And what that means is each individual neuron,

19:46

each individual brain cell, there's

19:48

it sort of forgets what it's doing,

19:51

and it's normally getting inputs from a bunch of other neurons. And

19:53

then in these down states, these low states,

19:55

everything goes quiet. And

19:58

the neuron just stops. And then

20:00

when it restarts, it's like, well,

20:02

what was happening? And it starts just behaving a

20:04

little bit randomly. And what

20:06

this means is that the overall

20:08

patterns of 4 flow and

20:11

communication throughout the brain are

20:13

broken. And that's what we really see at a whole

20:15

brain level. We see

20:17

an interruption of how different parts of the

20:19

brain speak to each other.

20:19

That's really interesting because you're saying that we

20:22

see that at your own level, at a at a brain

20:24

level, but also as we started the

20:26

program talking about having a chat with Hannah when she

20:28

just woken up from her last general and

20:30

aesthetic, it also works at a sort of personality

20:32

level. You've had a pause

20:34

in your personality and you wake up slightly

20:36

confused. Yeah. That's

20:37

right. And, yeah, your

20:38

neurons forget and you forget as well. There's a

20:40

nice parallel there. And she

20:43

seems to be talking about there being a

20:45

sort of threshold that there's lots of

20:47

variants. It seemed to be very

20:49

individualized at which point

20:51

the drug turns your brain

20:51

off. Yeah. I think that's interesting. It's not that

20:54

surprising, really. I mean, we all have different brains,

20:56

so it's very likely that we're

20:58

going to respond to anesthetics in different ways. I mean, that's part

21:01

of the art of Anesthesia that Fionio

21:03

was talking

21:03

about. You can't just

21:05

know exactly

21:05

how much anesthetic it's gonna take to keep

21:08

someone at that fine balance

21:10

between just enough and not

21:12

too much So looking for

21:14

a signature in the brain that can

21:16

tell us when someone is

21:18

anesthetized, when they've lost consciousness, just

21:21

at that threshold, that's that's an extremely useful

21:23

thing to be able to

21:24

do. That's an

21:25

interesting point there, Fiona, that Animal

21:28

makes. Do you notice this this this variation

21:30

in different

21:30

individuals, the level of anesthetic that they

21:33

each require? Yeah. Yeah. New people

21:35

are different with certainly,

21:37

it takes quite a long time for them to go off to sleep and they're quite

21:39

restless as they're going off to sleep whereas other

21:41

people will go to sleep quite

21:43

quickly. Seemingly with the same amount

21:45

of drug. So, yeah, probably not a variation. One

21:47

of the things that we know about pain

21:49

thresholds from the episode

21:52

back a few series ago with involved me being tortured

21:54

by Irene is that we know that 4

21:56

localized pain, for topical pain,

21:59

that people vary, and we know that my

22:01

colleague sitting across from me with her

22:03

ginger

22:03

hair. Ginger's need

22:04

more and asbestos. She's actually have a higher

22:06

pain threshold. That is is

22:08

supposed to be true. Yes.

22:09

No. That's local anesthetic. That's local

22:12

pain. Does it does this does this sort

22:14

of variation transfer into

22:15

general, into Britain, the brain waves that we're talking about?

22:17

I think

22:18

it's more around pain

22:20

than actual anesthesia. You

22:24

know what? Even so I need to

22:26

I need to can't be full of drugs

22:28

enough to knock out a small

22:29

horse. That's a Right. Yes.

22:32

Wow. Anyway, but that that individual

22:35

variation wanna go back to thinking about what's happening

22:37

in the brain, Anil, that that

22:39

that must give us clues to what the actual physiology,

22:41

what the neurochemistry, what

22:44

what neurochemistry is happening in the brain

22:46

when consciousness is switched off if there is so

22:48

much variance between

22:49

people. There's a lot of variance. I think

22:52

there's important levels to think about how an

22:54

aesthetics work. One is this very

22:56

basic level of what anesthetic

22:58

chemicals doing to the neurons or to the

23:01

synapses, the connections between neurons.

23:03

And here,

23:03

a really important

23:04

clue comes from the fact that anesthetics

23:07

don't just work on humans. They work on pretty

23:09

much anything that's

23:10

alive. They work on mice. They work

23:13

on insects. They work on

23:15

fruit flies.

23:16

They work on bacteria, and they even work

23:18

on plants. Now you have these

23:20

these plants called mimosaputica,

23:22

the so called don't touch me plant touch

23:25

me not plant. And if you touch it, please

23:27

retract. If you infuse

23:29

a plant like that with with

23:31

anesthetics, it loses the ability to respond.

23:33

So anesthetics at some

23:35

level work on something that's shared

23:37

across pretty much everything that's

23:39

alive. And

23:40

that might be, as we were talking about earlier, it might

23:42

be something very, very fundamental to do

23:44

with the

23:45

membranes of the surfaces of cells

23:47

and how their shapes are formed and so

23:49

on. No. Do you think do you

23:50

think we should start an instantizing carrots

23:53

before we cut into

23:53

them? Well, I

23:54

mean, I I think you should. Yeah.

23:57

I

23:57

think Yes.

24:00

Wow. Anyway, you wanna go back

24:02

to thinking about what's happening in the brain

24:04

that

24:05

is

24:05

it possible to even score levels

24:07

of consciousness? How do we actually do we is

24:09

there a metric? Do we how do we know

24:11

how conscious one is? There are

24:13

a number of metrics that as Fiano was

24:16

mentioning, that brain monitoring has been

24:18

happening in operating here. This is a very

24:20

long time to try and keep track of

24:22

the level of

24:23

anesthesia. And there's actually been some very

24:26

exciting research that has developed

24:28

new kinds of consciousness or

24:30

anesthesia meters

24:30

that go a little bit beyond just

24:33

measuring how strong

24:36

particular waves

24:36

are like these delta oscillations in

24:38

the brain.

24:38

So there are it's a kind of a new

24:41

generation of consciousness meters that

24:43

have been tested under anesthesia, but also

24:45

under other ways of losing

24:47

consciousness like sleep or in people with with

24:49

severe brain injury as well. And these

24:51

are all based on measuring the

24:53

complexity of the

24:55

brain dynamics under these

24:57

different

24:57

conditions. And by complexity, I mean

24:59

something very intuitive here. Like, so when

25:02

the brain is doing something, very

25:04

very predictable. Yeah. Just repeated

25:06

patterns over and over again, that's

25:08

very low complexity. And when the

25:10

brain is behaving completely randomly, like

25:12

every neuron during its own

25:13

thing, every neuron for itself.

25:15

That's also not very complex either.

25:19

Complexity in this sense is this balance between

25:21

simplicity in predictability and

25:24

randomness. And a new approach that's

25:26

that's been pioneered for the last twenty years

25:28

or so has been measuring

25:31

levels of brain complexity. And

25:33

one really good way of

25:35

doing this is a very impressive way I think it's very

25:37

it's very cool actually. Is by using a

25:39

combination of EG, which

25:41

we've already talked about, which measures the tiny

25:43

electrical field generated by

25:45

brain

25:45

cells. And a method

25:48

called TMS transcranial

25:50

magnetic stimulation. And this is

25:52

basically put a big magnet by

25:55

the

25:55

brain turn it on very briefly, and

25:57

it and it injects a pulse of energy

25:59

into the brain. And you don't notice

26:01

this. It just activates the

26:03

the brain through

26:04

the skull. And you can see the response

26:07

to the this activation in the EEG.

26:09

It's a bit like banging on the

26:11

brain with an electrical hammer and listening

26:13

to the echo. And what

26:16

you find is that by measuring

26:18

the complexity of the echo, is it is

26:20

it just like throwing a

26:22

stone into pond and there's a there's a single response, but

26:24

it all dies out very quickly. That

26:27

goes along with unconscious states.

26:29

But if the echo is complex, like you throw a

26:32

few stones into the pond and all these waves

26:34

are bouncing around and you just see this

26:36

complicated patterns, that

26:38

come and go over space and

26:40

time. That's indicative

26:42

of a conscious state. And what's

26:44

really exciting is that researchers have been able to put a

26:47

number to this complexities. And

26:49

this number seems to

26:51

be able to track conscious level,

26:53

both in anesthesia, but also in

26:56

sleep and and after brain injury and

26:58

and many other conditions

26:59

too. And this gives us a clue

27:02

about everyday consciousness as well. We

27:04

need this we

27:05

need these complex interactions where different parts

27:07

of the brain speak to each other

27:09

in different ways. In order to

27:11

be

27:11

conscious? So there's I

27:14

mean, in summary, still quite a lot we don't

27:16

understand. I think there's still a lot to

27:18

to I didn't mention this at the beginning of but

27:20

I'm actually having a general understanding tomorrow.

27:23

So I'm not sure whether I'm gonna be

27:25

more fearful after this

27:27

conversation or less. I think I'm gonna

27:29

ask, though, Fiona, for the 4 little drug that they give you

27:31

that makes you feel like you've had three gin and tonics

27:33

before I go in.

27:36

Thank you so much to our guests. Anoseth

27:38

and Theona Donald.

27:41

So Professor Fry,

27:43

when it comes to the question of how anesthetics

27:46

actually work. Can we say k solved?

27:48

Absolutely not doctor Rutherford. Real

27:50

doctors use them every day and no

27:52

one quite knows what magic chemistry is happening

27:54

in our

27:54

brains. We fully lose a sense of

27:56

self though, our consciousness is

27:59

erased temporarily. Anesthetics

28:01

involve blocking certain neurochemical

28:03

pathways and putting the brain into a

28:05

holding path of slow

28:06

brainwaves. But precisely

28:08

how anesthesia actually works remains

28:10

one of science's great mysteries.

28:12

I woke

28:16

up during an operation once.

28:19

Did you? Mhmm. AGA general answer. No. It was

28:21

like twilight. I think that's what they call it.

28:23

It's essentially where they they give you local

28:26

anesthetic and then they heavily sedate you.

28:28

It's lower risk.

28:31

So this is when I had some

28:33

an operation on my lymphedema, which

28:36

I spoke about a number of times in

28:38

Anyway, what happened was I

28:40

woke up and then I wanted

28:42

to see what was going on so I sat up

28:45

and, like, had a look. And I don't really remember seeing

28:47

that much, but I do remember the reaction

28:49

of everyone in the

28:50

room. They were like, oh, no. And then they pushed me

28:53

back down. Crikey. I mean, we

28:55

should reassure the listeners that that

28:57

is a different

28:58

process. Yeah. Yeah. Yeah. To general

29:01

understanding, And it was very it was I it's subcutaneous.

29:03

Right? So -- Yeah. -- it's very superficial.

29:05

I probably could have got away without even being

29:06

sedated. That's sedation

29:07

process. Just 4 being a

29:08

wimp. Still not III

29:11

also file that in the category of suboptimal.

29:14

There's so many interesting things about

29:16

this. And I I know that, you know, is one of

29:18

the sciences great mysteries that

29:20

this thing that use day in, day out, and

29:23

and we don't have a fundamental

29:25

understanding of what what is happening

29:27

in this in this weirdness. There's

29:29

there's another thing that we didn't quite get to in the show, which I

29:31

think is fascinating. Irene and Tracy was

29:33

talking about the the natural variants, how

29:35

people are different, and people's brains are

29:38

different. And a

29:40

while back, I was involved in a project talking

29:42

about the genetic differences and reactions

29:45

to anesthetics. And

29:47

there's a really interesting category.

29:49

There's one particular anesthetic

29:51

which is which is used

29:53

acetylcholine eschar as it's called and it's

29:56

it's addressing a system which

29:58

is so you can intubate people. So during

30:00

anesthetics, you need to get a tube down down

30:02

their throat and our throats generally don't like that

30:03

happening. Mhmm. So you give a local

30:06

anesthetic which suppresses the muscles, and

30:08

it normally lasts about

30:10

five

30:10

minutes. But in the late seventies,

30:13

early eighties in India, A doctor

30:15

started to notice that some of their

30:18

patients were going in

30:20

for voluntary operations.

30:23

And they were not

30:25

waking up from their anesthetics for up

30:27

to five or six hours.

30:29

And then

30:30

they wake up and they're absolutely fine. Right?

30:32

So it wasn't clinically problematic.

30:35

So they went through this process of trying to

30:37

understand which bit of the anesthetic cocktail

30:39

was causing this, and this particular group of people was

30:41

reacting to. And it was in fact this

30:44

substance alconate's bit of the

30:46

anesthetic cocktail

30:46

mix, which is first suppressing the

30:49

the gag reflex.

30:50

And then they noticed that it was only happening

30:52

in one particular group of people

30:55

and that so in India,

30:57

there's still a lot of cost based.

30:59

In dogami, it's cool. So, you know,

31:02

different groups of people that

31:04

don't necessarily into breed with each other.

31:05

Mhmm. That's a very bad way to explain it. Yeah.

31:08

But it may I mean, it makes it I mean, like,

31:10

there's all sorts of extremely

31:13

problematic cultural history

31:15

associated with this, but but ultimately

31:18

ultimately you have groups that that

31:20

tend to marry and have children within their

31:22

own

31:22

groups. Exactly. That's

31:23

exactly right. That's a much better way of explaining

31:25

that. You're welcome. You know

31:26

what? I can see she's just hearing on Eugenics.

31:30

Wait a minute. Just explain genetics

31:32

and race to me. I think you did. Anyway,

31:34

so what what was noticed was it was

31:36

one particular

31:36

cast. It was It was Just just to be

31:38

clear, anyone that doesn't know how

31:41

heavily addicted Adam is when

31:42

he's on. It would work on genetic. He has a

31:44

radio four series at the moment called Bad Blood on you

31:46

on you tanks. I'm just popping the

31:48

word Eugenics in as a

31:49

law. It's important to just say it makes a

31:52

plan eighty percent of my work here. Anyway,

31:54

the point is that this one group of people at

31:56

Voicea Cast, which is like a

31:58

merchant cast in in India,

32:01

all of the people that were were weren't waking up

32:03

for 456

32:05

hours. From this particular pseudo code, anesthetics,

32:07

were all from this one

32:10

group. And so it was

32:12

it was worked out that there

32:14

was a particular genetic mutation in the chemical pathway,

32:16

the metabolic pathway, which

32:19

is processing this

32:21

particular anesthetic, and it only

32:23

exists in this one particular group. So

32:25

when you notice things like that, you

32:27

can you can go back through history and work

32:29

out when that mutation occurred, and we think it occurred

32:31

about two thousand years ago,

32:33

existed for two thousand years

32:35

with absolutely zero effects because

32:37

no one was being anesthetized with

32:39

Colleen Estores. Oh,

32:41

crazy. And then all of a sudden, you see, oh,

32:43

there you go. So that's a When we

32:45

when we were talking about that, I was thinking about that because both

32:48

Anil and I have Indian

32:50

heritage, but

32:52

I I wonder what what mutations have

32:54

already happened that it that that that

32:56

have not yet revealed themselves because

32:58

we're not yet drinking the draft

33:01

Yeah. That that that will show themselves

33:03

to be. Well, alcohol is is a good

33:05

example of that because there's there's

33:07

natural variation in alcohol

33:10

dehydrogenase. Which is the

33:12

the enzyme that processes our goal.

33:14

And it's distributed in wildly

33:16

different ways around the

33:16

world. And it only has

33:19

an effect if you drink if you

33:21

drink.

33:21

And that and that's a big part of the history of

33:24

scientific racism in fact. Oh, it's

33:26

happened. I've got to it.

33:28

Really took fifty five series.

33:31

Yeah. But that's for another time. Should we

33:33

do

33:33

some Curio correspondence? Curio

33:35

correspondence, which comes before, 4. Wait.

33:38

Wait. Wait. How much?

33:40

Correspondence. What are

33:43

you

33:43

saying? It's just

33:46

been suggested that one of our things.

33:49

Emma, our studio manager who's just that. I

33:51

don't know Adam and I, we've

33:53

now got to the stage of curiousness is where we've done

33:55

an episode on every conceivable

33:57

thing, including an episode on

33:59

singing. And if you'd listened to that, you know that it is not

34:01

wise to ask, particularly

34:04

me. But

34:04

also, Adam Kisill bang on about being

34:07

4 after I that used to a chorister. Let's do

34:09

correspondence. There's no there's a theme tune. So I

34:11

I left a pause then for the theme tune, but there

34:13

isn't one. There isn't

34:16

one. So a couple of episodes ago,

34:18

we did a Phantasia, the concept of the blind minds I knew.

34:20

Inability to visualize in your

34:22

head -- Mhmm. -- and both

34:25

was to the tests online, and

34:28

then I came up with a hyper

34:29

visualizer, and I think you were

34:31

pretty good visualizer. An

34:34

AFAN average. I think I was I was

34:36

bang average. A Fantastic Put

34:38

People. I can't I still can't remember how

34:40

to pronounce that. Fantastic. Yeah.

34:41

That's right. It's fantastic.

34:42

But lots and lots of people were

34:45

stunned -- Yeah. -- to realize

34:47

as this program went out that they

34:49

themselves were a fantastic and had thought

34:51

that as people all around them were saying picture in your

34:53

mind's eye, they thought it was metaphorical and

34:56

didn't realize that

34:58

other people could actually

35:00

picture things in their minds

35:01

eye. And there were so many

35:04

so many emails that came in about

35:06

this. Yeah.

35:06

And Twitter was awash with it as well. It's quite

35:08

it's very interesting now because it's not a well known phenomenon. And yet,

35:10

three percent,

35:11

you know. Yeah. And

35:14

didn't know about it. But

35:16

anyway, so there there isn't an

35:18

afantasian network. So if if if this is

35:20

something that you've come across, then then get in touch

35:22

with those guys. If you just just share your

35:24

experiences and support, Yeah. Loads of

35:25

correspondence. This one from Martin Christmas,

35:28

rather lovely. My wife, Kate, and I have just had

35:30

the most incredible evening.

35:32

I had no idea that A Frantasia even

35:34

existed and had always assumed

35:36

the phrase mind's eye was a metaphor.

35:38

We've had a lovely evening laughing

35:40

about how our our mad differences

35:42

that we never even knew despite being

35:44

married for ten

35:45

years. I wonder if it's, you know, you could

35:47

actually explain some of the joys

35:50

and maybe not joys in

35:52

relationships when

35:53

when when when discovering that you've got a condition that no one knew

35:56

about. How extraordinary. I mean,

35:57

it's like one in thirty people. One

35:59

in thirty three

36:01

percent? percent. Don't call me professional

36:04

mathematician for

36:04

nothing. Okay. Should

36:05

we take care of

36:08

the week?

36:10

Asperious, totally unique heat. Rutherford

36:15

and fries, Curio.

36:19

Of the week.

36:26

We got a letter in from Greg Wilson. proposed

36:28

an exchange of badges,

36:31

badges, badges -- Oh, right. -- not the

36:33

mammals. Right. I don't.

36:36

As I know

36:36

that you are fans of Matt Damon.

36:38

How does he know

36:41

that? I think

36:42

he's And we

36:44

ever saw them again.

36:45

One getting. You may recognize us as

36:47

the employer of his character

36:49

in goodwill hunting. You may recognize us as

36:51

the employer of his character in

36:54

goodwill. What

36:54

Okay. So either right. So he was

36:57

a builder at one point? Yes.

37:00

But he also was at university. He was at a

37:01

janitor. Janitor. In the university? Yes.

37:04

And he kept solving mathsy things on

37:06

glass, which is how mathematicians aren't

37:08

sold. No. No.

37:09

No. No. No. No. No. No. No. No. No.

37:11

There was no glass. It was a mirror

37:13

in

37:13

the toilet. Do you know what? I've got to be

37:15

careful about correcting you about

37:18

films because I did it in an episode recently and I was

37:19

wrong. Where are you? Yes. Okay.

37:21

There is

37:22

a mirror. I accept there's a mirror, but I don't think that counts

37:24

as glass. That's a mirror. No.

37:26

I think he does it with with a

37:27

chalkboard. Right. You're right. And I think it's and

37:29

4 you're right

37:30

about the mirror. Let's let's call that one square

37:32

draw. What did you get wrong?

37:35

It was when I said, cherry cherrypot fire was about

37:37

the four minute mile, it wasn't. It was

37:39

about Harold Abrahams running in the Olympics

37:41

years before Roger Bannister. I can see you

37:43

looking confused about whether you should be smug that I got it wrong and

37:46

absolute lack of interest in what I'm

37:47

saying. No. It was more that I thought it was

37:49

about Roman races.

37:52

So something like

37:54

the niche

37:55

the, like, nuanced differences between your

37:57

two things. But men on

37:59

Twitter did enjoy pointing it out.

38:02

So men on Twitter.

38:03

Okay.

38:03

Are you ready? Yeah.

38:04

What is it? Mhmm. Okay. Mhmm. Okay. It's

38:06

too bad. Here you go. Here you go. And

38:10

they say e over c

38:11

squared. Oh,

38:12

I know what it is. What it is? I know what

38:14

it is. Let me see if I can work it out.

38:17

So so we should read it out. It's an equation. It says

38:19

e over c squared times square root

38:21

of minus one times

38:24

p v over NRR

38:26

icon I do

38:27

this? Mhmm. Right. E is energy. But

38:30

what's an

38:30

equation with e

38:31

and c squared

38:34

in it? Equals m c squared

38:35

to c is the square the the speed of light? Yes. So

38:37

what's the other letter

38:38

in the m? Yeah. So m equals

38:41

E0C squared.

38:43

M equals e over c

38:45

squared. Yeah.

38:45

The middle bit, square root of minus one

38:48

is an imaginary number, which is

38:50

next week's show. Correct? The square root of minus

38:52

one is minus one. No, one?

38:54

I don't know. It's I. Come on. Oh,

38:56

I. Yeah. Imagine number. Thank you, Descartes.

39:00

So IEMI.

39:02

Now what's p v over in our

39:04

PV pressure? Think think

39:07

what's the question? E equals m c squared.

39:09

No. PVPV over n. What's what's actually Is

39:12

that an an electrical one? No.

39:15

TV pressure. Volume, voltage. Yes. Volume.

39:18

Volume. Oh, is this hook's

39:19

lock? Oil's lock. No. Oh,

39:20

I don't know. And the ideal gas

39:22

sequase embarrassing. What are -- -- radius of a

39:25

circle,

39:25

of a spear. I don't know if I think

39:27

Why don't we stop? What

39:31

else do you get with gases? What other

39:33

smell? Look,

39:33

it says MI, and they're an

39:36

employer. MI An

39:39

employer, five. Mission

39:42

Impossible. M is

39:44

equals

39:45

a t come on. Equals NRT

39:48

time. MIT. MIT. MIT.

39:50

MIT. Matt Damon worked. Will Hunting

39:52

worked at MIT? Well

39:55

done, sir. Look.

39:57

Frankly, the thing that I've

40:00

noticed that the most about this is

40:02

that Greg

40:04

spent seventeen dollars and eighty five cents in airmailing

40:06

list to

40:07

us. Yeah. Thank you, Greg. I

40:09

mean, I struggled to get through it with a

40:11

lot of hand holding

40:14

by my colleague there. But I enjoyed that. I enjoyed

40:16

that. That's cool. Also, neither us work

40:18

at MIT. You can't have a badge.

40:21

Yes, you

40:23

can. I'm saying that you can. She's

40:25

the VTO. She used the VTO. We have to we

40:27

do have something else that's connected to Matt

40:30

Damon because Duncan McMillan shared the surprising news with

40:32

us. Every

40:33

year, Steven Soderberg.

40:36

Soderberg. Who's

40:38

that?

40:38

Steven Soderberg is like one of the great living directors. Yeah.

40:41

Yeah. He's made amazing

40:43

films like that all of the oceans,

40:45

films, oceans eleven. That's

40:48

great. Twelve and thirteen, not so much. But also complete

40:50

classics like, I believe you made magic

40:52

mic, which is brewing film, and

40:55

the one about the pandemic

40:58

that happened before the pandemic where Glynis

41:00

Contagion --

41:01

Mhmm. -- is superb director. What's

41:03

going on

41:03

here? Aaron Brokovich. He made

41:06

Aaron Brokovich. That's cool. Oh,

41:06

he's cool. Oh, and and and one of the films he made with Matt Damon was

41:09

the the biography of

41:11

Leverage, which is

41:13

Excellent. Okay. Cool. Alright. So anyway, this

41:16

guy, this this man, mister

41:18

Stephen, he summarizes everything that he's read and watched every

41:20

year, and he consumes a lot. the

41:22

twenty ninth of January last year, he

41:24

read the complete guide to absolutely

41:26

everything, which is mine in and Adam's

41:29

book. No way.

41:31

I thought you

41:31

should know if you don't already. We didn't know. Adam

41:34

very clearly did not know. That

41:36

is no way. I clicked on

41:38

the link. Holy smokes.

41:40

He's like one of my favorite directs of

41:42

all

41:42

time. Is he? And he's read

41:44

one of your books.

41:47

One hour book. Wow. And you were in Where

41:50

is that? I can't see that. I can't see that. I

41:52

can't see that. I've made it. Yeah.

41:55

You know what? That's cool. That is re that is

41:57

really cool. You know what

41:59

though? What I've noticed is that he's got

42:01

basically something every single day, apart from

42:03

the day when he red

42:05

art book where he

42:08

doesn't have anything for five

42:10

entire days

42:11

afterwards. He was just Yeah. Washing. Yeah. Lying it to wash it.

42:13

I'm looking over the list here. It's really interesting.

42:15

But also, a couple of days later, he watched

42:17

Beavers and But Head do

42:20

the universe. Lovely,

42:22

Skye. Steven Soderberg, if

42:24

you're listening to this show, having read our

42:27

book, I love your

42:28

films. And I want to be your friend. And

42:30

I'm sorry to know who you were, but I I mean, I'm I'll happily be your friend

42:32

too. Watch jaws on

42:35

the fourth of September. 4. Adam's

42:38

just reading the list of everything that he's read now.

42:41

There's a question. Okay. This is let's

42:43

let's let's leave it We'll

42:45

leave in the post. Next week, we're talking about the

42:47

maturing numbers. Bay one. Glass onion, he

42:50

watch. All about eef RRR. That was

42:52

really good. The thick of it. Oh my

42:53

god. I love

42:53

this so much. That is the best things

42:56

that ever happened

42:58

to me. I'm

43:06

Paris Lays. Welcome to the

43:08

flip side from BBC

43:10

4 Radio In each

43:13

episode, I'll tell two stories from opposite sides

43:15

of the coin. A new

43:18

science to ask questions

43:20

about elements of the

43:22

human

43:22

experience. That we sometimes take for 4.

43:26

Turns out that this person that

43:27

I sublet my

43:30

apartment to he was, you know, a scammer. I see now

43:32

I am the

43:33

person that I was when I was

43:35

on the Internet at thirteen. It's

43:38

lies and it's covered with lips

Rate

Join Podchaser to...

  • Rate podcasts and episodes
  • Follow podcasts and creators
  • Create podcast and episode lists
  • & much more

Episode Tags

Do you host or manage this podcast?
Claim and edit this page to your liking.
,

Unlock more with Podchaser Pro

  • Audience Insights
  • Contact Information
  • Demographics
  • Charts
  • Sponsor History
  • and More!
Pro Features